1) Adams J A. A closed-loop theory of motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1971; 3(2): 111-50.
2) Schmidt R A. A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review. 1975; 82(4): 225.
|
3) Fitts P, Posner M. Human performance. Monterey, California: Brooks/Cole; 1976. P. 244-45.
|
4) Masters RSW. Knowledge, knerves and know-how-the role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. Brit J Psychol. 1992; 83: 343-58.
|
5) Masters R, Maxwell J. The theory of reinvestment. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2008; 1(2): 160-83.
|
6) Bliss C B. Investigations in reaction time and attention. Studies from the Yale Psychology Laboratory. 1892; 1: 1-55.
|
7) Boder D P. The influence of concomitant activity and fatigue upon certain forms of reciprocal hand movements and its fundamental components. Comparative Psychology Monographs. 1935; 11: 78-79.
|
8) Deikman A J. DE automatization and the mystic experience. In C. T. Tart (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness. New York: Wiley; 1969; 29(4): P. 23-43.
|
9) Wulf G, McNevin N, Shea CH. The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2001; 54(4): 1143-54.
|
10) Maxwell J P, Masters R, Kerr E, Weedon E. The implicit benefit of learning without errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A. 2001; 54(4): 1049-68.
|
11) Baddeley A D, Wilson B A. When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the problem of error elimination. Neuropsychologia. 1994; 32: 53–68.
|
12) Gentile A M. Implicit and explicit processes during acquisition of functional skills. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Therapy. 1998; 5: 7–16.
|
13) Berry D C, Dienes Z. Implicit learning: Theoretical and empirical issues. Hove, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1993; 1: 153.
|
14) Baddeley A D, Hitch G. Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation. New York: Academic Press; 1974. p. 47–89.
|
15) Poolton J, Masters R, Maxwell J. Passing thoughts on the evolutionary stability of implicit motor behavior: Performance retention under physiological fatigue. Consciousness and Cognition. 2007; 16(2): 456-68.
|
16) Zhu F, Poolton J, Wilson M, Maxwell J, Masters R. Neural co-activation as a yardstick of implicit motor learning and the propensity for conscious control of movement. Biological Psychology. 2011; 87(1): 66-73.
|
17) Lam W K, Masters R S, Maxwell J P. Cognitive demands of error processing associated with preparation and execution of a motor skill. Consciousness and Cognition. 2010; 19(4): 1058-61.
|
18) Schmitz X, Bier N, Joubert S, Lejeune C, Salmon E, Rouleau I, et al. The benefits of errorless learning for serial reaction time performance in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2014; 39(2): 287-300.
|
19) Sanli E A, Timothy D. What roles do errors serve in motor skill learning? An examination of two theoretical predictions. Journal of Motor Behavior Ahead-of-Print. 2014; 329-37.
|
20) Reber A S. The cognitive unconscious: An evolutionary perspective. Consciousness and Cognition. 1992; 1(2): 93-133.
|
21) Abernethy A B, Hanrahan S J, Kippers V, MacKinnon L, Pandy M. Biophysical foundations of human movement. 2en ed. Illinois: Human Kinetics; 2005. P. 272-4.
|
22) اشمیت ریچارد ای، لی تیموتی دیی. یادگیری و کنترل حرکتی. مترجمان: حمایتطلب رسول، قاسمی عبدالله. چاپ اول. تهران: انتشارات علم و حرکت؛ 1387. ص 58ـ57.
|
23) Heuer H, Hegele M. Generalization of implicit and explicit adjustments to visuomotor rotations across the workspace in younger and older adults. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011; 106: 2078 –85.
|
24) Wulf G, Schmidt R A. Feedback-induced variability and the learning of generalized motor programs. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1994; 26: 348–61.
|